Usage-based_models_of_language

Usage-based models of language

Usage-based models of language

Linguistics approach / theory


The usage-based linguistics is a linguistics approach within a broader functional/cognitive framework, that emerged since the late 1980s, and that assumes a profound relation between linguistic structure and usage.[1] It challenges the dominant focus, in 20th century linguistics (and in particular in formalism-generativism), on considering language as an isolated system removed from its use in human interaction and human cognition.[1] Rather, usage-based models posit that linguistic information is expressed via context-sensitive mental processing and mental representations, which have the cognitive ability to succinctly account for the complexity of actual language use at all levels (phonetics and phonology, morphology and syntax, pragmatics and semantics). Broadly speaking, a usage-based model of language accounts for language acquisition and processing, synchronic and diachronic patterns, and both low-level and high-level structure in language, by looking at actual language use.

The term usage-based was coined by Ronald Langacker in 1987.[2] Usage-based models of language have become a significant new trend in linguistics since the early 2000s.[1] Influential proponents of usage-based linguistics include Michael Tomasello, Joan Bybee and Morten Christiansen.

Together with related approaches, such as construction grammar, emergent grammar, and language as a complex adaptive system, usage-based linguistics belongs to the wider framework of evolutionary linguistics. It studies the lifespan of linguistic units (e.g. words, suffixes), arguing that they can survive language change through frequent usage or by participating in usage-based generalizations if their syntactic, semantic or pragmatic features overlap with other similar constructions.[3] There is disagreement as to whether the approach is different from memetics or essentially the same.[4]

Disciplinary roots

West coast cognitive functionalism

West Coast cognitive functionalism (WCCF) played a major role in the creation of the usage-based enterprise. Firstly, a crucial point in WCCF was Eleanor Rosch’s paper on semantic categories in human cognition,[5] which studied fuzzy semantic categories with central and peripheral concepts. Subsequently, Robin Lakoff (1987) applied these concepts to linguistic studies. For usage-based models of language, these discoveries legitimized interest in the peripheral phenomena and inspired the examination of the ontological status of the rules themselves.[6] Secondly, WCCF focuses on the effects of social/ textual context and cognitive processes on human thought, instead of established systems and representations, which motivated the study of external sources in usage-based language research. For example, in analyzing the differences between the grammatical notions of subject vs. topic, Li and Thompson (1976), found that the repetition of certain topics by a speech community resulted in the surfacing and crystallization of formal properties into syntactic entities, namely the subject.[7][8][9] This notion of syntax and morphology being an outcome of pragmatic and cognitive factors[10] was influential in the development of usage-based models. Thirdly, the WCCF methodology of linguistic typology[11] is similarly practised in usage-based models, in collecting data from real communicative contexts and analyzing them for typological regularities. This highlights an important aspect of usage-based research, the study of methods for the integration of synchrony and diachrony.

Langacker’s Cognitive Grammar

The term ‘usage-based’ was coined by Ronald Langacker in 1987, while doing research on Cognitive Grammar. Langacker identified commonly recurring linguistic patterns (patterns such as those associated with Wh- fronting, subject-verb agreement, the use of present participles, etc.) and represented these supposed rule-governed behaviours on a hierarchical structure. The Cognitive Grammar model represented grammar, semantics and lexicon as associated processes that were laid on a continuum, which provided a theoretical framework that was significant in studying the usage-based conception of language.[12] Consequently, a usage-based model accounts for these rule-governed language behaviours by providing a representational scheme that is entirely instance-based, and able to recognize and uniquely represent each familiar pattern, which occurs with varying strengths at different instances. His usage-based model draws on the cognitive psychology of schemata,[13] which are flexible hierarchical structures that are able to accommodate the complexity of mental stimuli. Similarly, as humans perceive linguistic abstractions as multilayered, ranging from patterns that occur across whole utterances to those that occur in phonetic material, the usage-based model acknowledges the differing levels of granularity in speakers’ knowledge of their language. Langacker's work emphasizes that both abstract structure and instance-based detail are contained in language, differing in granularity but not in basic principles.

Bybee's Dynamic Usage-based framework

Bybee’s work[14][15][16][17] greatly inspired the creation of usage-based models of language. Bybee’s model makes predictions about and explains synchronic, diachronic and typological patterns within languages, such as which variants will occur in which contexts, what forms they will take, and about their diachronic consequences. Using the linguistic phenomenon of splits (when a word starts to show subtle polysemy, and morphological possibilities for the originally single form ensue), Bybee proves that even irreducibly irregular word-forms are seen to be non-arbitrary when the context it occurs in is taken into consideration in the very representation of morphology. Simultaneously, she shows that even seemingly regular allomorphy is context-sensitive. Splits also aligns with the idea that linguistic forms cannot be studied as isolated entities, but rather in relation to the strength of their attachment to other entities.[18]

Schmid's Entrenchment-and-Conventionalization model

Hans-Jörg Schmid’s "Entrenchment-and-Conventionalization" Model offers a comprehensive recent summary approach to usage-based thinking.[19] In great detail and with reference to many sub-disciplines and concepts in linguistics he shows how usage mediates between entrenchment, the establishment of linguistic habits in individuals via repetition and associations, and conventionalization, a continuous feedback cycle which builds shared collective linguistic knowledge. All three components connect linguistic utterance types with their respective situative settings and extralinguistic associations.

Frequency explanation

Advocates of usage-based linguistics including Joan Bybee and Martin Haspelmath argue that statistics of language usage depend on frequency. For instance, it is argued that the English verb tell always has two arguments ('tell something to someone') unlike the verb sell, which more frequently only has a direct object in actual language usage ('sell something'). It is hypothesized that such differences in the recurrence of the indirect object depend on statistical learning based on the language usage encountered by the individual. Jae Jung Song argues that the frequency explanation is circular—certain patterns are often used by people because they are frequent—and that the explanation of frequency issues must be found outside themselves.[20]

Constructions: Form-meaning pairings[21]

Constructions have direct pairing of form to meaning without intermediate structures, making them appropriate for usage-based models. The usage-based model adopts constructions as the basic unit of form-meaning correspondence.[22][23][24][25] A construction is commonly regarded to be a conventionalized string of words. A key feature of a grammar based on constructions is that it can reflect the deeply intertwined lexical items and grammar structure.

From a grammarian perspective, constructions are groupings of words with idiosyncratic behaviour to a certain extent. They mostly take on an unpredictable meaning or pragmatic effect, or are formally special. From a broader perspective, construction can also be seen as processing units or chunks, such as sequences of words (or morphemes) which have been used often enough to be accessed together. This implicates that common words sequences are sometimes constructions even if they do not have idiosyncrasies or form.[26][27][28] Additionally, chunks or conventionalized sequences can tend to develop special pragmatic implications that can lead to special meaning over time. They can also develop idiosyncrasies of form in a variety of ways.

  • It drives me crazy.
  • The death of his wife the following year drove him mad.
  • This room drives me up the wall.

Adjectives shown here include crazy, mad, and up the wall, which are semantically related to the word drive. In exemplar models, the idea that memory for linguistic experience is similar to memory for other types of memories is proposed. Every token of linguistic experience impacts cognitive representation. And when stored representations are accessed, the representations change. Additionally, memory storage can store detailed information about processed tokens during linguistic experience, including form and context that these tokens were used. In this model, general categories and grammar units can emerge from linguistic experiences stored in memories, as exemplars are categorized by similarity to each other. Contiguous experiences such as meaning and acoustic shape are also recorded to be linked to each other.

Constructions as chunks

By these means repeated sequences become more fluent. Within a chunk, sequential links are graded in strength based on the frequency of the chunk or perhaps the transitions between the elements of a chunk. A construction is a chunk even though it may contain schematic slots, that is, the elements of a chunk can be interrupted.

Memory storage requires links to connect idiomatic phrases together. In chunking, repeated sequences are represented together as units which can be accessed directly.[29][30] Through this, repeated sequences are more frequent. Sequential links are assessed in strength based on the frequency of the chunk or transitions between elements within a chunk. Additionally, the individual elements of a chunk can link to elements in other contexts. The example of ‘drive someone crazy’ forms a chunk, however items that compose it are not analyzable individually as words that occur elsewhere in cognitive representation. As chunks are used more frequently, words can lose their associations with exemplars of the same word. This is known as de-categorialization.

See also


References

  1. von Mengden, Ferdinand; Coussé, Evie (2014). "Introduction. The role of change in usage-based conceptions of language". Usage-Based Approaches to Language Change. Studies in Functional and Structural Linguistics. Vol. 69. pp. 1–20. doi:10.1075/sfsl.69.01men. ISBN 978-90-272-1579-6.
  2. Serafima Gettys, Patricia Bayona, Rocío Rodríguez. "From a Usage-Based Model to Usage-Based Instruction: Testing the theory" (PDF). International Journal of Education and Human Developments. 4: 50.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  3. Christiansen, Morten H.; Chater, Nick (2008). "Language as shaped by the brain" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 31 (5): 489–558. doi:10.1017/S0140525X08004998. PMID 18826669. Retrieved 2020-12-22.
  4. Blackmore, Susan (2008). "Memes shape brains shape memes". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 31 (5): 513. doi:10.1017/S0140525X08005037. Retrieved 2020-12-22.
  5. Boyes-Braem, P; Johnson, D; Gray, W.; Mervis, C.B.; Rosch, E. (1976). "Basic objects in natural categories". Cognitive Psychology.
  6. Iverson, G.K., Lima, S.D.; Corrigan, R.L. (1994). The reality of linguistic rules. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  7. Givon, T (1979b). "From discourse to syntax: Grammar as a processing strategy". In T. Givón (ed.). Discourse and Syntax. Vol. 12. New York: Academic Press. pp. 81–109.
  8. Givon, T. (1979c). On understanding grammar. New York: Academic Press.
  9. Givon, T (1989). "Modes of knowledge and modes of processing. The routinization of behavior and information". Mind, Code, and Context: Essays in Pragmatics. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 237–268.
  10. Hopper, P. J. (1997). "When 'Grammar' and Discourse Clash: The Problem of Source Conflicts". In J. Bybee; J. Haiman; S. A. Thompson (eds.). Essays on Language Function and Language Type. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. pp. 231–247.
  11. Greenberg, J.H. (1960). "A quantitative approach to the morphological typology of language". International Journal of American Linguistics. 26 (3): 178–194. doi:10.1086/464575. S2CID 144261944.
  12. Murkherjee, J. (2004). "Corpus Data in a Usage-Based Cognitive Grammar". In K. Aijmer; B. Altenberg (eds.). Advances in Corpus Linguistics: Papers from the 23rd International Conference on English Language Research on Computerized Corpora (ICAME 23) Göteborg 22-26 May 2002. Language and Computers: Studies in Practical Linguistics. Vol. 49. Amsterdam: Rodopi. pp. 85–100.
  13. Schank, R.C.; Abelson, R.P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  14. Bybee, J. L. (1985). Morphology: A study of the relation between meaning and form. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
  15. Bybee, J. L. (2001). Phonology and language use. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  16. Bybee, J. L. (2006). Frequency of use and the organization of language. New York: Oxford University Press.
  17. Bybee, J. L.; Perkins, R.D.; Pagliuca, W. (1994). The evolution of grammar: tense, aspect, and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  18. Luce, P.A.; Pisoni, D.B; Goldinger, S.D. (1990). "Similarity neighborhoods of spoken words". In G. T. M. Altmann (ed.). Cognitive Models of Speech Processing: Psycholinguistic and Computational Perspectives. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 122–147.
  19. Schmid, Hans-Jörg (2020). The dynamics of the linguistic system : usage, conventionalization, and entrenchment (First ed.). Oxford. ISBN 978-0-19-254637-1. OCLC 1139239358.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  20. Song, Jae Jung (2012). Word Order. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9780521872140.
  21. Filmore, Charles J. (1988). "The mechanisms of Construction Grammar". Proceedings of the 14th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. Vol. 14. pp. 35–55. doi:10.3765/bls.v14i0.1794. {{cite book}}: |journal= ignored (help)
  22. Croft, William (2001). Radical Construction Grammar: Syntactic Theory in Typological Perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  23. Goldberg, Adele E. (2006). Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalizations in Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  24. Goldberg, Adele E. (1995). Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  25. Goldberg, Adele E.; Casenhiser, Devin (2006). "English Constructions". In Bas Aarts; April McMahon (eds.). The Handbook of English Linguistics. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 343–55. doi:10.1002/9780470753002.ch15. ISBN 9780470753002.
  26. Bybee, Joan L.; Eddington, David (2006). "A usage-based approach to Spanish verbs of becoming". Language. 82 (2): 323–55. doi:10.1353/lan.2006.0081. S2CID 145635167.
  27. Bybee, Joan L.; Hopper, Paul J. (2001). Frequency and the Emergence of Linguistic Structure. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
  28. Ellis, Nick C. (1996). "Sequencing in SLA". Studies in Second Language Acquisition. 18: 91–126. doi:10.1017/S0272263100014698. hdl:2027.42/139863. S2CID 17820745.
  29. Newell, Allen (1990). Unified Theories of Cognition. Cambridge: MIT Press. ISBN 9780674921016.

Share this article:

This article uses material from the Wikipedia article Usage-based_models_of_language, and is written by contributors. Text is available under a CC BY-SA 4.0 International License; additional terms may apply. Images, videos and audio are available under their respective licenses.